Skip to main content

Conspecific neighbors and kinship influence egg cannibalism in the green lacewing, Chrysoperla carnea (Stephens)

Abstract

Background

Chrysoperla carnea (Stephens) (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae) is an illustrious predator that performs cannibalism upon facing small and defenseless conspecifics. Eating preference of C. carnea larvae was investigated between conspecific and host (Sitotroga cerealella) eggs. Early instars of C. carnea express significant preference toward conspecific eggs over host eggs. Experiments were carried out to examine the role of conspecific neighbors in execution of cannibalistic decision. These cannibalistic behaviors of larvae were also investigated through locomotion bioassays using the video tracking software ANY-mazeĀ®.

Results

Larvae commit low egg cannibalism when alone but express higher cannibalism in the presence of conspecific larvae in the neighborhood. The role of relatedness in egg cannibalism was investigated when larvae were offered a choice between conspecific eggs from related and unrelated parents. Larvae did a discrimination between eggs of their own parents and those of other parents.

Conclusion

Behavioral bioassays showed attraction toward conspecific eggs and the eggs of unrelated parents. This study emphasizes the significance of informational and descriptive cues that modulate the cannibalistic expression in C. carnea.

Background

Cannibalism is usually supposed as an abnormal behavior of killing and ingestion of all or part of conspecifics which was generally ignored until the 1970s (Simpson et al. 2018). Since last several decades, it has been revealed that cannibalism is a prevalent feeding scheme that can be induced by ecological factors such as a low food availability and a high population density (Vijendravarma et al. 2013; Arvaniti et al. 2019). Cannibalism occurs generally across many taxa in an environment, and it is supposed to lessen the real numbers where cannibalism occurred because the data are not from systematic surveys (Richardson et al. 2010). Cannibalism involves fitness benefits and costs for individuals; the victim generally incurs only costs, unless by being consumed it can uplift its inclusive fitness. The primary benefit for cannibal is to get food and kill a potential competitor (Van den Beuken et al. 2019). Furthermore, the cannibal may get a better balance of nutrients that are essential for growth and development than who consume other heterospecifics (Tigreros et al. 2018).

Predatory insects are more common in consumption of their conspecifics where they are naturally allowed to kill and consume live prey due to their physical and physiological adaptation (Simpson et al. 2018). Predators habitually target weak life stages, such as eggs or younger nymphs/larvae, possibly to avoid injury. However, this behavior emanates with several other possible costs, such as injuring and killing siblings, thereby lessening the indirect fitness of the cannibal, as well as heightening the chances of acquiring the infection of deleterious parasites and pathogens (Pizzatto and Shine 2011).

Organisms encounter conspecifics for committing filial cannibalism by two possible mechanisms. First, phenotypic cues they possessed help to kin recognition directly (e.g. paper wasp (Ropalidia marginata), honeybees (Apis mellifera) and desert isopods (Hemilepistus reaumuri) can differentiate between sibling and non-sibling using these cues or labels like odors (Neff and Sherman 2003). Second, conspecific recognition may be done using association between probabilities of encountering offspring and non-phenotypic cues (LissĆ„ker and Svensson 2008). For example, female red flour beetle, Tribolium castaneum, uses locational cues to identify conspecifics. Before getting engaged in cannibalism, they leave the area into which their offspring disperse, thereby lowering the possibility of eating their own youngā€™s (Pfennig 1997). If the dispersal is limited, existence of reproductive fellows in the close neighborhood may act as a cue to recognize their own offspring (Manica 2004). Regardless of kin recognition, offspring that have less likelihood of survival may be more prone to cannibalized by parents, as this let the parents to cannibalize some of them later, when condition may become more adverse, instead of losing all at once.

The expression of cannibalistic behavior in generalist predator such as the green lacewing, Chrysoperla carnea (Stephens) (Neuroptera: Chrysopidae), has been investigated toward conspecific eggs by larval instars. Initially, the present hypothesis tested was that whether larvae commit egg cannibalism in the presence of host S. cerealella eggs or not, and is there any significance in their consumption. Furthermore, an increase in the egg cannibalism rate of C. carnea larvae was tested, when conspecific larvae were present in the neighborhood. Also, examined whether direct association influences egg cannibalism when larvae offered related and unrelated conspecific eggs. Keeping in view of the high searching aptitude of C. carnea larvae, behavioral locomotion bioassays were performed to better understand the filial relationship and cannibalistic preference.

Methods

Green lacewingā€”collection and rearing

C. carnea adults were captured from Tandojam (location: 25.4203Ā° N, 68.5445Ā° E) during year 2019, as commonly found in cotton, sugarcane and mango orchard. Specialized LED-based light traps were installed in various fields to attract lacewing trichromatic vision (Gaglio et al. 2017). The collected adults were transferred to rearing cages (23.5ā€‰Ć—ā€‰23.5ā€‰Ć—ā€‰20.5Ā cm) made of crystal acrylic sheets (1Ā cm gauge). Adults were reared on an artificial diet consisting honey, sugar, yeast and distilled water (1:2:1:2), replaced daily and supplied ad libitumĀ (Sohail et al. 2018). Rearing cages were provided black linen cloth at the top for egg laying (Sohail et al. 2019). Collected C. carnea eggs were transferred in black cover cloth and fed on frozen Angoumois grain moth, Sitotroga cerealella eggs (Pasini et al. 2018). On hatching of first instars, larvae were sealed in 2 inches polypropylene transparent straw/tubes with three pin aeration holes to avoid cannibalism (Sohail et al. 2019). Pupae were then separated from tubes and transferred on glass Petri plates (6Ā cm diameter) for emergence. The life stages were maintained at 25ā€‰Ā±ā€‰1Ā Ā°C and 60ā€‰Ā±ā€‰5% RH and 15:9 (L/D) for further studies.

Cannibalism of C. carnea

Experiment 1, conducted during 1ā€“2 October 2019, tested cannibalism rates in different instars of C. carnea. Aim was to produce an independent test to compare the rate of cannibalism of each instar. All the 3 instars were kept in polypropylene transparent tubes (5Ā cm long) and starved for 10Ā h to standardize their appetite (Sohail et al. 2020). Starved 1st, 2nd and 3rd instar larvae received 20, 40 and 60 conspecific eggs in Petri dishes (60ā€‰Ć—ā€‰15Ā mm), respectively. Eight individuals of each instar were assessed for their cannibalism. At the end of 24-h isolation, the cannibalism rate of each individual instar was measured.

Cannibalism in the presence of host

Experiment 2, conducted during 14ā€“18 October 2019, tested whether the presence of host eggs modulates the expression of egg cannibalism. Egg cannibalism of each instar was evaluated, when S. cerealella eggs were also provided in equal amount. The arena consisted of 9Ā cm Petri dish with 90-mm filter paper at base. A lid over the arena contained the mesh net (mesh sizeā€‰=ā€‰1.15 mm2) in the center for air flow. The arena was provided C. carnea eggs in one half and S. cerealella eggs in another half. After 24Ā h of starvation, the different numbers of egg ratio (C. carnea: S. cerealella) offered to each larval instar were: 1st instar 15:15; 2nd instar 30: 30; and 3rd instar 45: 45. Eight individuals of each instar were assessed for their cannibalism. Under the hypothesis of decreasing cannibalism rate in the presence of host eggs, data were recorded of egg cannibalism within 24Ā h.

Cannibalism in competition

Experiment 3, conducted during 11ā€“22 November 2019, tested whether C. carnea larvae exhibit greater rate egg cannibalism when they found company of conspecifics in their neighborhood. Another aim was to characterize the relationship between egg cannibalism by focal larva with respect to the number of neighboring larvae. On day 0 (10 November), individual C. carnea larvae were isolated in 2 inches polypropylene transparent tubes with excess frozen S. cerealella eggs. Isolation was practiced to standardize the larval perception of native conspecific density before treatment. On day 1 (11 November), prior to place them in the arena, each separated individual was placed at starvation for 24Ā h. The test arena comprised of a customized glass Petri plate (60ā€‰Ć—ā€‰20Ā mm) separated along diameter in equal half by a mesh net (mesh sizeā€‰=ā€‰1.15 mm2). Focal larvae was placed any side in the half, while other half either received 1, 2, 4 and 8 conspecific larvae (Nā€‰=ā€‰8, 8, 8 and 8, respectively) or left empty as control (Nā€‰=ā€‰8). S. cerealella eggs were provided to the larvae expect the focal larvae, which were given known amount of conspecific eggs to evaluate cannibalism rate. The mesh net prevented larvae from crossing over but permitted the focal larvae to sense neighboring conspecific larvae. Focal larvae were allowed to cannibalize the eggs for 24Ā h. Cannibalized eggs were identified by their absence and empty or collapsed chorion. As C. carnea eggs hatched in about 3ā€“4Ā days; thus, eggs couldnā€™t have hatched during study. One-way analysis of variance, followed by HSD test, was used to compare egg cannibalism rate with different numbers of neighboring individuals. Paired t test (pā€‰>ā€‰0.05) was used to compare all treatments with control.

Filial cannibalism

Experiment 4, conducted during 2ā€“12 December 2019, aimed to evaluate the probability of filial cannibalism modifies the manifestation of cannibalism. Egg cannibalism of C. carnea larvae was tested on eggs from their own parents and eggs from other random couple. Here the tested hypothesis was to check the cannibalism choice of the larva between conspecific eggs of related and unrelated parents. Three arenas were constructed to execute this experiment. Each one constructed by the method described for experiment 2. The 2nd instar larvae were placed in each arena for 24Ā h isolation to standardize with arena surroundings. After isolation, different amounts of eggs offered to larva in each arena were: 1st arena 30 conspecific related eggs; 2nd arena 30 conspecific unrelated eggs; and 3rd arena 15 conspecific related eggsā€‰+ā€‰15 conspecific unrelated eggs. In 3rd arena, related and unrelated eggs offered to larva at different halves. This experiment was replicated 8 times with 36 number of larvae. After 24Ā h, numbers of related and unrelated eggs cannibalized by the larvae were counted.

Behavioral response

The olfactory behavioral response was examined during 16ā€“28 December 2019, intended to track C. carnea larvae in a dual choice Y-maze bioassay (Fig.Ā 1). The maze was made up of glass with three arms (one release; two novel). Novel arms were provided with wells for diet/treatment application. All arms were closed with 80 mesh sieve to avoid either escape of larva or to stick on diet/treatment. A single C. carnea larva was released through the release arm and its behavior was observed for up to 10Ā min. For each diet combination, 20 larvae were tested. After each bioassay, the maze was wash with soap and then with 70% ethanol and dried in an oven at 121Ā Ā°C. The following 2 combinations were test: (1) C. carnea eggs vs. S. cerealella eggs; (2) eggs form related parents vs. eggs from unrelated parents. First combination was tested against 1st, 2nd and 3rd instar C. carnea larvae, while second was tested just 2nd instar larva. Completely randomized design was used to analyze the data of 20 larvae per diet combination. Larval behavior was recorded with the digital camera (NikonĀ® D-5200, with Nikon lens 10ā€“24Ā mm f/3.5ā€“4.5, Tokyo, Japan) fitted at a height of 40Ā cm above the arena. Video recordings were analyzed using video tracking software (ANY-mazeĀ® 6.1, Stoelting CO, USA). The Y-maze arena apparatus was set to record readings of only novel arms. Path tracking, distance traveled, speed and time spent in each novel arm were recorded using 20 larvae of each instars. The novel arm where C. carnea larvae spent more than half of the total time was considered preferred and the other arm that retained the larvae less than half of the time was marked non-preferred.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Layout of arena used for video tracking

Statistical analysis

Cannibalism rate of each instar was compared using one-way analysis of variance and HSD test. Difference in consumption of (C. carnea and S. cerealella) eggs and consumption of eggs (related and unrelated) was analyzed using paired t test (pā€‰>ā€‰0.05) per instar. Likewise, in behavior response bioassay, difference in parameters (distance, time and speed) was analyzed using paired t test (pā€‰>ā€‰0.05). All statistical analyses were performed using statistical software ā€œSPSSā€, version 16.0.

Results

In experiment 1, the significance difference (fā€‰=ā€‰26.1, dfā€‰=ā€‰23, pā€‰<ā€‰0.00) was found in egg cannibalism of different larval instars of C. carnea (Fig.Ā 2). Second and third instar larvae expressed 1.6 and 2.3-fold higher egg cannibalism, respectively, and found more voracious toward conspecific eggs than the first instar larvae.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Conspecific egg cannibalism by larval instars of C. carnea. Each bar represents mean egg consumption (Ā±ā€‰SEM, where Nā€‰=ā€‰8 for each bar). Difference in letters above the bars indicates significant (pā€‰<ā€‰0.05)

In experiment 2, in the presence of host eggs, the rate of egg cannibalism varied greatly with a significant difference among the first and second instar larvae (Fig.Ā 3). Earlier instar larvae (first instar (tā€‰=ā€‰5.7, dfā€‰=ā€‰14, pā€‰<ā€‰0.00) and second instar (tā€‰=ā€‰3.5, dfā€‰=ā€‰14, pā€‰=ā€‰0.003)) were committed significant egg cannibalism, regardless of the presence of host eggs, indicating the attraction to conspecific eggs was not influenced by the presence of host eggs. However, the third instar larvae were equally consumed conspecific eggs and host eggs (tā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ā€‰1.6, dfā€‰=ā€‰14, pā€‰=ā€‰0.132).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Horizontal bars indicate average number of eggs consumed by specific C. carnea instars. All horizontal lines indicateā€‰Ā±ā€‰SEM with Nā€‰=ā€‰8 for each bar. Asterisk beside the bars indicates significant difference between consumed egg proportion of C. carnea or S. cerealella by C. carnea instars

In experiment 3, both second and third instar larvae were evaluated for egg cannibalism in the presence of conspecific larvae (Fig.Ā 4). Lone focal second instar larvae expressed low egg cannibalism (20.6ā€‰Ā±ā€‰1.1). However, focal larvae in the presence of neighboring larvae 1, 2, 4 and 8 expressed 1.2, 1.3, 1.3 and 1.8-fold increase in egg cannibalism, respectively. Likewise, during the assay of third instar, focal larvae held alone continued to exhibit low cannibalism (33.9ā€‰Ā±ā€‰2.5), but those held in the presence of conspecific larvae 1, 2, 4 and 8 become 1.1, 1.3, 1.6 and 1.6-fold more cannibalistic, respectively. Egg cannibalism changed significantly with increase in numbers of neighboring larvae (second instarā€‰=ā€‰fā€‰=ā€‰31.6, dfā€‰=ā€‰39, pā€‰<ā€‰0.00; third instarā€‰=ā€‰fā€‰=ā€‰23.6, dfā€‰=ā€‰39, pā€‰<ā€‰0.00). Results of comparison between control and each number of neighboring larvae were found significant in both (second and third) instar, except when held with single neighbor.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Number of eggs (meansā€‰Ā±ā€‰SE, where Nā€‰=ā€‰8 for each bar) cannibalized by C. carnea focal larvae (Aā€‰=ā€‰2nd instar, Bā€‰=ā€‰3rd instar) with or without choice. Horizontal lines with circular ends indicate comparison of egg cannibalism between focal larvae with (i.e. one, two, four or eight) neighboring larvae, separately. Difference in letters above the bars indicates significance (pā€‰<ā€‰0.05) with respect to control (0 neighbor), collectively

In experiment 4, nonsignificant difference was recorded in egg cannibalism when C. carnea larvae were held in two separate arenas for each type (related and unrelated) of eggs (pā€‰=ā€‰0.736) (Fig.Ā 5). However, conspecific eggs of the other parents (unrelated) were significantly more preferred to cannibalize than the eggs of the same parents (related) (pā€‰<ā€‰0.00).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Number of related or unrelated eggs (meansā€‰Ā±ā€‰SE, where Nā€‰=ā€‰8 for each bar) cannibalized by C. carnea focal larvae with or without choice. Horizontal lines with circular ends indicate their comparisons with or without choice

Behavioral response

In the test combination of C. carnea eggs vs. S. cerealella eggs, time proportion spent by the first instar of C. carnea in both novel arms was statistically similar (tā€‰=ā€‰0.14, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.894) and no preference was found toward different eggs (Figs. 6 and 7). Likewise, nonsignificant difference was found in speed (tā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ā€‰0.79, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.441) and as well as in the distance (tā€‰=ā€‰1.4, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.187) traveled by first instars in both novel arms provided with C. carnea and S. cerealella eggs. However, behavioral preference to C. carnea eggs was recognized by the second and third instar larvae, where they spent more than half of the total time in the particular novel arm. Speed of second instar larvae remained similar in both novel arms (tā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ā€‰0.68, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.502) but the significant difference was detected in third instar larvae (tā€‰=ā€‰7.9, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰<ā€‰0.00). Distance traveled by second instar was significantly higher (tā€‰=ā€‰6.2, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.00) in the novel arm provided with C. carnea eggs than the other arm with S. cerealella eggs, while nonsignificant difference was found in the distance traveled by third instar larvae (tā€‰=ā€‰0.8, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.430).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Track (A) and heat (B) map visualizing the path followed and time spent by 1st (I), 2nd (II) and 3rd (III) instar individual C. carnea in movement area for ten minutes. Vertical arm is a release arm, where Y-maze provided with C. carnea eggs at left arm and of S. cerealella eggs at right arm. Blue and orange spots are starting and ending point, respectively. Data were analyzed using video tracking software (ANY-mazeĀ®, Stoelting CO, USA)

Fig. 7
figure 7

Graph presents distance traveled (vertical bars) time (line with triangle ends) and speed (line with circular ends) of 1st (I), 2nd (II) and 3rd (III) instar C. carnea in movement area for ten minutes. All vertical lines indicateā€‰Ā±ā€‰SEM with Nā€‰=ā€‰20, where Y-maze provided with eggs of C. carnea and S. cerealella

Furthermore, in the second (eggs from related parents vs. eggs from unrelated parents) test combination, second instar C. carnea larvae preferred eggs of unrelated parents over eggs of unrelated parents (tā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ā€‰13.06, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.00) (Figs. 8 and 9). Speed (tā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ā€‰2.03, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.66) and distance (tā€‰=ā€‰0.56, dfā€‰=ā€‰19, pā€‰=ā€‰0.58) traveled in both novel arms were found similar.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Track (A) and heat (B) map visualizing the time spent of 2nd instar individual C. carnea in movement area for ten minutes. Vertical arm is a release arm. Y-maze provided with eggs of related parents at left arm and of un-related parents at right arm. Blue and orange spots are starting and ending point, respectively

Fig. 9
figure 9

Graph presents distance traveled (vertical bars), time (line with triangle ends) and speed (line with circular ends) of 2nd instar larvae of C. carnea in movement area for ten minutes. All vertical lines indicateā€‰Ā±ā€‰SEM with Nā€‰=ā€‰20, where Y-maze provided with eggs of related and unrelated parents

Discussion

Cannibalism in C. carnea is a predominant phenomenon that can be assessed as to its significance for both the individual and the population. Keeping in view its nutritional need, a critical life stage of the larva is from hatching to invading of its debut prey. During that stage, larval mobility is comparatively low and tolerance to hunger is poor (Pitwak et al. 2016). After hatching, young larvae start scanning the instant surroundings that increase the probability of discovering their conspecific eggs. Three to four eggs per day may extend the likelihood of survival even in the absence of other prey. Therefore, larval searching may be significantly prolonged and with it, its probabilities of discovering other prey. Egg cannibalism reduces the number of developing individuals but also consider as a mean for enabling survival until the other prey is encountered rather than as providing diet for entire growth.

Nutritional requirements of a larval instar vary depending on the individualā€™s stage of development that regulate the prey consumption in predator. In the present study, earlier instar C. carnea larvae cannibalized less eggs than did later instar larvae, as one would expect given the size difference among the larval instars. Furthermore, metabolism also typically increased in late instar larvae, this usually due to the insectā€™s increased activity (Kerkut 2013).

In experiment 2, early instars (first and second) of C. carnea were committed egg cannibalism significantly more even in the presence of host eggs (S. cerealella). Likewise, third instar larvae were equally consumed conspecific eggs and host eggs. Video tracking was used to assess their cannibalistic behavior. Lateral instars of C. carnea preferred to stick with the novel arm, containing conspecific eggs rather than host eggs. Similar observations were documented that some of insects prefer to eat conspecifics instead of natural food (Barros-Bellanda and Zucoloto 2005). This behavior may be triggered by the requirement of good food quality or by scarcity of high population density of potential competitors. Food is essential for living beings to continue life, but preference of conspecifics over natural food may have direct influence on increase insect fitness. Conspecific eggs are known to be more nutritious to developing larvae, who could consequently be helped by cannibalizing eggs when host or host eggs are rare or of poor quality (GagnĆ© et al. 2002). The preference for conspecific eggs depicted that larvae can discriminate among different foods. Furthermore, cannibals are found to have prolonged survival, higher weight, higher rate of development and/or fecundity as compared to the non-cannibals (Tanahashi and Togashi 2009). Barrosā€Bellanda and Zucoloto (2001) reported that the pierid, Ascia monuste, cannibal larvae gain higher weight and survival rate than non-cannibal ones. Likewise, Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata (Wiedemann) committed an egg or larval cannibalism to propitiate themselves in improved growth and development (Zucoloto 1993). This demonstrates that although natural food is present, insects may take their conspecifics for nutritional benefits. Regarding non-nutritional factors, cannibalism resultant from high population density. Individual that cannibalizes eliminates possible conspecific predator and a potential competitor (Polis 1981). Eating conspecifics preferably may lead to reduce the population size, which make food per capita more abundant, and increase the chances to survive and develop more rapidly. These reasons may involve in changing feeding preference to conspecifics instead of other food source.

In experiment 3, second and third instar larvae were found less cannibalistic when alone become highly cannibalistic in the presence of neighboring larvae. Committing of this cannibalism is usually due to amplified aggression for resource limitation when the density of cannibals is high (Wise 2006). Here, C. carnea larvae might use presence and absence of neighbors to assess the resource competition, challenged from other conspecifics in the future. In such case, C. carnea larvae would be more cannibalistic in the presence of conspecifics, because they are scared to get starve in future. The second possibility is, if the conspecifics consumed these eggs, they would gather more strength and power. So, being weak fellows, focal larvae would have low chances to survive under these circumstances. One more thing might have been hypothesized that nearby siblings could effect in the raised level of stress which may lead to elevated metabolism and consequently higher cannibalism.

Therefore, focal larvae should also be expected to respond to the neighborhood as it could be experienced by other conspecifics. Hence, the resource competition appeared in this experiment to elucidate the observed upsurge in egg cannibalism in reaction to the conspecific neighbors.

In experiment 4, the results revealed that the second instar larvae of C. carnea were more likely to eat unrelated conspecific eggs than related in no choice situations, which indicated a reluctance to cannibalize related eggs, perhaps suggesting kin recognition. Likewise, the results of video tracking bioassay also showed significant preference toward unrelated eggs in no choice environment, which resulted in more retention time in novel arm provided with unrelated eggs. Joseph et al. (1999) reported that the third instar of Asian ladybird beetle, Harmonia axyridis (Pallas), preferentially eat unrelated conspecifics instead of relatives. The neonates of ladybird predators, Coccinella transversalis and Propylea dissecta, avoided eating-related conspecific eggs and preferably consumed unrelated conspecific eggs (Omkar et al. 2004). The present study suggests C. carnea larvae responded differently toward related and unrelated eggs and had an adequate mechanism of kin recognition. This mechanism can be conceptually partitioned into 3 phases: production of recognition cues or labels; perception and interpretation of labels; and appropriate actions accordingly (Pfennig 1997). These cues may be intraspecific chemical odors, which have direct or indirect basis of genetic material (Roberts et al. 2003). Chemicals present on the egg surface may be influential for preference/avoidance by cannibals and may be benefitted to discriminate among related and unrelated conspecific eggs (Hemptinne et al. 2000).

Conclusions

The study demonstrated that C. carnea larvae were committed egg cannibalism even in the presence of host eggs. It may depend on the type of host eggs. Future work is needed to check the cannibalism in the presence of eggs of different hosts. Cannibalism that is controlled by the presence of conspecific is a unique behavior that may initiate density-dependent cannibalism. Likewise, it is more often related with conditions when resources are limited. The behavioral response described here might upsurge cannibalism even when resources are sufficient, augment the circumstances under which cannibalism may be effective. The study suggests that the presence of conspecific C. carnea larvae induces focal larvae to become more cannibalistic toward conspecific eggs. Furthermore, an expression of C. carnea larvae toward related and unrelated conspecific eggs provides the best evidence of kin recognition. Obtained data set was based on laboratory studies. An empirical research must be needed in the natural environment to address the issues like: (1) dietary qualities of host to avoid cannibalism; (2) influence of interspecific competition on cannibalism; and (3) the bases of cues or labels that limit or promote cannibalism.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

Ā°C:

Celsius

RH:

Relative humidity

cm:

Centimeter

mm:

Millimeter

HSD:

Honesty significance difference

References

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MS planned the research project, wrote, revised and edited the manuscript. QAS helped to conduct the experiments and wrote the manuscript. MUA and IR made critical revisions to the manuscript. RM supervised the research and helped to analyze the data. All authors have read and agreed to the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mubasshir Sohail.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that there is no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sohail, M., Soomro, Q.A., Asif, M.U. et al. Conspecific neighbors and kinship influence egg cannibalism in the green lacewing, Chrysoperla carnea (Stephens). Egypt J Biol Pest Control 31, 140 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41938-021-00485-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41938-021-00485-4

Keywords